Memory – the
ultimate unreliable narrator.
When
we remember events, especially after some time has passed since these events,
we do not recall them as if seeing them on film. We recall edited, modified
versions of what actually happened. What we remember is influenced by our
personalities, by our moods, by our interpretations. Long term memory can be fickle.
Neuroscientist
Professor Susan Greenfield, was speaking on ‘Saturday Live’ on BBC Radio Four recently.
She talked about how unreliable our memories can be over time. She stated that
as we age, and as we are changed by experience, our memories are also altered.
We
reframe events to fit where we are now. This is not the same as dishonesty.
It’s just a fact of how our brains work. We are easily convinced that what we
remember is true. And to some extent our recollections are true. They are true to who we were and are, but they’re not
necessarily accurate. Of course memories can be accurate, but accuracy seems to
depend on how involved we were in the event being recalled, how active we were
in the scenario, how significant it seemed to us at the time. We process and
rationalise our experiences and store edited and manageable versions of the
facts.
And
while it would seem logical to assume that the more traumatic an event is, the
more likely the details will be seared in the memory as ‘flashbulb’ moments, scientific
studies have shown this is not necessarily the case.
*Flashbulb
memories was a term coined by researchers Roger Brown and James Kulik after
they researched people’s memories of the assassination of President John F
Kennedy. Brown and Kulik asserted that the vividness with which people recalled
seeing and/or hearing of the assassination was because such a traumatic event
produced ‘picture-like’ clarity. However they had not interviewed their
subjects immediately after the killing, but several years later. They were
impressed with the vividness of the reported memories, but they had no first
memories to compare these with.
But
the whole notion of the accuracy of
flashbulb memory was first challenged by Professor Ulric Neisser from Cornell
University. After the explosion of the space shuttle, Challenger, in 1986, he set
up an investigation into the supposed phenomenon. He interviewed a group of
college students less than twenty-four hours after the disaster about where
they were and what they were doing when they heard about the explosion. He then
re-interviewed them thirty months later. Twenty-five percent of them were
mistaken about every detail. Only seven percent received perfect recall scores.
A
similar study carried out by Jennifer Talarico and David Rubin who recorded the
9/11 experiences of fifty-four students at Duke University in the US three days
after that terrible event, found that seven months later those memories had
faded at the same rate as everyday ones.
However,
further studies carried out by psychologist and neuroscientist Dr Tali Sharot
have shown that the closer to and the more involved a person is in an incident,
the more vivid their memory will be and the more convinced they’ll be of its
accuracy. Sharot suggests that it’s vividness and not accuracy of memory that serves us well. She uses the example of
being attacked at night in a park. If this happens to you, you are unlikely to
enter that park in the darkness ever again. Details of what the attacker looked
like, or the precise time of the attack are less likely to be stored. What
matters is that a confident memory of the episode serves as a constant reminder
not to repeat the behaviour that led to the trauma.*
Memory
seems to have evolved in humans to aid future planning i.e. to record salient
and useful points, but not to lay down detailed photographic records of the
past.
So,
if memory is not clear cut, scientific or completely reliable, in what way is it
significant for writers?
For
writers of fiction, the unreliability of memory doesn’t matter. The memories of
novelists, short story writers and poets are vital wells from which to draw.
Our writing comes from our experience. That experience can be real, imagined or
a blend of both. Even a completely made-up, surreal or fantastical component
has to have its roots in some form of remembered experience. The mind, memory
and imagination are all inextricably linked. This is never more true than when
they’re used in the creative process. Memory will fire the imagination. The
writer’s mind will have to make sense of the outcome of that firing for the
reader’s benefit. But there will be no need for the original memory to be
accurate, no need for the author to interrogate it. For the novelist memory is
a useful, even vital, tool and it’s there to be distorted, played around with,
reframed.
Writers
of diaries or journals who are recording actual events will, however, aim to be
more accurate in their recollection. But even although their recording will be
close to immediate, they will filter what they write. They’ll choose which
memories to put in and which ones to leave out. They’ll then process and
reflect on the experiences they do decide to include.
However,
you would assume that writers of memoir would by definition be recounting
accurate memories of a life lived. But
they too, like the rest of us, are susceptible to distorted remembrances. And even if they weren’t, there is no way a
whole life can be written down. A memoir has to be episodic. The written
version of a life has to be an edited version. The memory of the author will be
selective, reflective and subjective and I suspect that’s what makes the memoir
interesting and very human.
In
writing, as in life, memories have enormous power. They have power to comfort,
to torture, to silence, to protect and to inspire. And, also as in life,
writers use memory to inform, to persuade and to entertain. And, yes, as in
life, it’s up to those on the receiving end of these reworked memories to
decide on their usefulness, meaning and truth.
* A full account of these studies along
with many other fascinating insights into the workings of the human mind can be
found in ‘The Optimism Bias’ written by Tali Sharot and published by Robinson.
ISBN 978- 1-78033-263-5
Anne Stormont is a writer and teacher.
She can be a subversive old bat but maintains a kind heart. As well as writing
for this fine organ, she writes fiction for adults – mainly of the female-of-a-certain-age persuasion – and
for children. She blogs at http://annestormont.wordpress.com – where you can find out lots more about
her.
No comments:
Post a Comment